Pronunciation model: New Zealand English

View the key for New Zealand English here.

The relatively homogenous varieties of English spoken in New Zealand (Gordon & Maclagan 2008) in one sense make this one of the more straightforward varieties to represent. Historical influences from settlers from across the British Isles and Australia have resulted in patterns and contrasts clearly similar to these varieties in many respects, with Southern English by far the most significant phonological influence (although a semi-rhotic Scottish-influenced quality remains in Southland and parts of Otago on the South Island).

The base New Zealand English model is therefore largely based on the phonetic qualities described by Bauer & Warren (2008), and influenced by the contrasts portrayed in Deverson & Kennedy’s (2005) The New Zealand Oxford Dictionary (NZOD). The vowel keywords and their symbols are given in the table below.

 KEYWORD Symbol KEYWORD Symbol KEYWORD Symbol KEYWORD Symbol
 KIT ɪ FLEECE iː NEAR iə HAPPY i
 DRESS e GOOSE uː SQUARE eə LETTER ə
 TRAP ɛ PALM ʌː CURE uə RABBIT ə
 BATH ʌː START ʌː FACE æe ADDED ə
 LOT ɒ NURSE ɜː PRIDE ɑe BEAUTIFUL (ə)
 CLOTH ɒ NORTH oː VOICE oe PIANO i
 STRUT ʌ FORCE oː MOUTH æu AGO ə
 FOOT ʊ THOUGHT oːGOAT ʌu BESIDE ə + i

Of the compromises required in developing this model, one is deserving of particular mention. Bauer & Warren note how lip-rounding is relatively weak in New Zealand English, but there an auditory impression of lip-rounding can still be heard even where the lips are not strongly rounded. This is particularly apparent in words such as nurse. Rather than introduce a new symbol to represent this particular phonetic feature, or use a rounded vowel symbol which would misrepresent lip positioning, the most appropriate symbol within the OED’s existing symbol set was instead for the unrounded vowel /ɜː/. This reflects that there is a difference in quality from the British /əː/, but the compromise is that it is conflated with the Australian equivalent.

In general, New Zealand English is not a rhotic variety, so /r/ sounds pattern more similarly to British English than American English, but ‘flapping’ of /t/ to /d/ (bitter as /ˈbɪdə/) is commonplace and reflected consistently between vowels, and between vowels and syllabic consonants. The exception to this is in words of Māori origin, where flapping is dependent on the common degree of anglicization.

Despite the consistency across much of the New Zealand English variety, its model is unique amongst OED models in having a distinct pronunciation subset. Words of Māori origin are treated slightly differently from the rest. Although it is not the intention to represent a distinct Māori English (as described by Warren & Bauer 2008) nor adopt pronunciations simply reflective of the Māori language, the changing cultural and socio-political scene in New Zealand appears to have resulted in many European-origin speakers adopting less anglicized pronunciations of Māori terms, which the standard pronunciation model is insufficient to capture. Considerable evidence can be found on public video-hosting sites of speakers fluently interspersing their New Zealand English speech with Māori-approximating pronunciations of particular words. As Bauer & Warren suggest, these can be considered instances of code-shifting, i.e. the speaker is no longer speaking ‘English’ but a different language. Speakers who use non-native forms may adopt forms which are closer or more distant from the Māori, with ‘nativist’ and ‘assimilationist’ degrees of anglicization (although there are intermediate points). The pronunciations of Māori-origin words are given according to the patterns in the grid below, compiled from various sources including from the natural patterns of the OED speakers. In addition to these, it is worth noting that /r/, usually an approximant sound in New Zealand English, is usually a tap when pronounced in words of Māori origin, but unlike the flapping of /t/ is treated as an allophone of /r/ rather than resembling /d/. Consequently there is no transcription difference, but the audio is likely to reflect this.

It should be noted that not all sources display the macrons (the lines above the longer vowels) and they do not presently appear in the headwords of OED entries, but they are frequently given in the etymology sections of such words. Where uncertain, the pronunciations follow the macrons as given by the Te Aka Māori Dictionary of Moorfield (2022).

Māori SpellingNZE Form
aʌ (very occasionally ɒ), usually ə if unstressed
āʌː
estressed: e, occasionally æe unstressed: e, i and/or ə
ēstressedː æe, occasionally iː unstressedː æe, e and/or i
ii, rarer ə variant (unstressed word-medial) – /i/ taken to always encompass /ɪ/ variants
ī
oo or ɒ, sometimes ʌu
ōoː, sometimes ʌu
uu, ʊ if more reduced
ū
ngword-initially n; elsewhere ŋ
whf
aiɑe (PRIDE)
aeɑe (PRIDE), sometimes with bracketed 2nd segment (diphthong seemingly weakened for many speakers in some contexts, e.g. before /r/, as in ‘haere mai’)
oe, oioe (VOICE) (in some contexts oe may be /we/ or /wæe/)
aoæu (MOUTH)
auʌu (GOAT), sometimes also æu (MOUTH, the common Brit/US vowel)
euoften juː
iaiə (before another vowel sometimes just i, as in ‘kia ora’)
eaeə or æeə
eiæe
ua
ouʌu (GOAT)
āeɑe (PRIDE)

Diphthong vowels, where two vowel qualities are glided between but within the same syllable (as in British face or voice), are also a feature of Māori-origin pronunciations, but it is not always straightforward to determine these situations versus those where vowels are produced in sequence over two syllables. Review of these patterns is ongoing, with further revisions to be expected.

 Sources